Influence of Trap Colour on the Capture of Codling Moth (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae), Honeybees, and Non-target Flies

Influence of Trap Colour on the Capture of Codling Moth (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae), Honeybees, and Non-target Flies

Knight, Alan L

INTRODUCTION

Optimizing trap design is vital in developing a useful monitoring system for codling moth, Cydia pomonella L. (Knight and Christianson 1999). The effectiveness of a variety of sticky and non-sticky trap types have been reported (Knodel and Agnello 1990, Vincent et al. 1990), but until recently, a sticky cardboard white or cream color wing trap has been the standard for monitoring codling moth in the western United States (Riedl et al. 1986). Knight et al. (2002), however, found that either a delta or diamond-shaped trap was more effective than the standard wing trap in laboratory flight tunnel and in field trials. We believe the delta-shaped trap has now become the most widely used trap for monitoring codling moth in Washington State. Unfortunately, early in the season the delta-shaped trap constructed from white corrugated plastic consistently catches non-target flies and honeybees, Apis mellifera L. Trap contamination by flies and honeybees requires that the trap’s sticky liner be replaced more frequently and thus adds to the costs of monitoring codling moth.

Colour is an important factor influencing the foraging behaviour of honeybees (von Frisch 1967). Honeybees can differentiate six colour ranges between 300 and 650 nm, i.e. ultraviolet to yellow light (Burkhardt 1964). Variable degrees of contamination of monitoring traps by honeybees and bumblebees Bombus spp., due to differences in trap colour have been reported (Hamilton et al. 1971, Gross and Carpenter 1991, Meagher 2001). In general, white and yellow traps are attractive and green traps are unattractive to Apoidea species due to their differences in spectral reflectance between 380 and 550 nm (Mitchell et al 1989).

The influence of trap colour on the capture of some noctuid pests in sex pheromonebaited traps has been well studied (McLaughlin et al. 1975, Mitchell et al. 1989). However, the importance of the visual stimuli provided by traps in these two studies of night-flying moths contrasted sharply. McLaughlin et al. (1975) found that traps with low spectral reflectance were more effective in capturing Trichoplusia ni (Hubner) and Pseudoplusia includens (Walker), while Mitchell et al. (1989) found that such traps were less effective with Anticarsia gemmatalis Hubner and Spodoptera frugiperda (J.E. Smith). Trap colour has not previously been reported to be a specific factor influencing the capture of codling moth. The objective of our study was to evaluate the influence of trap colour on the attractiveness and selectivity of delta-shaped traps for codling moth, honeybees, and non-target flies.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Description of traps. White delta traps (Suterra, Bend, OR) were left unpainted or painted with one of five high gloss paints (Krylon , Cleveland, OH): Spring Grass green #2327, Pumpkin orange gloss #2411, Banner Red Gloss #2108, Ivory gloss #1504, and Gloss white #1501. The three darker colours were characterized based on value, chroma, and hue (Munsell Book of Colour 1976): green (4, 8, 5G), red (4, 14, 5R), and orange (6, 14, 2.5YR).

Spectral Reflectance. Trap samples (100 cm2) were scanned with a Perkin-Elmer Lambda-9/19 spectrophotometer (Wellesley, MA) by Avian Technologies (Wilmington, OH). Trap surfaces were scanned at wavelengths from 360 to 830 nm with a monochromatic slit width set at 2 nm and operated at a scan rate of 120 nm / min.

Experiments 1 and 2. Two experiments were conducted in a 5-year-old ‘Red Delicious’ apple orchard, Malus domestica (Borkh), (mean (SE) tree height = 2.2 (0.2) m) situated 15 km east of Moxee, Washington (46°40’N, 120°05’W) at the U.S.D.A. Experimental Farm during 2003. This orchard was situated 0.6 km east of a large dairy farm. Bloom in the apple blocks at the farm occurred from 25 April – 15 May. In the first study (24 – 28 April) delta traps were not baited with a sex pheromone lure. Traps in the second study (5-12 May) were baited with the Biolure 10X codling moth lure (Suterra, Bend, OR). Ten traps of each colour were placed in a completely randomized design in each experiment. Unsexed, laboratory-reared codling moth adults (n = 5,000) were released into the orchard prior to the start of experiment 2 only.

Experiment 3. A third experiment was conducted to compare the attractiveness of the unpainted white and the green-painted delta traps for codling moth. This study was conducted in a 10-ha 30-year-old mixed block of ‘Red Delicious’ and ‘Golden Delicious’ situated 5 km north of Moxee, Washington (46°33’N, 120°23’W). Mean (SE) tree height in this orchard was 4.3 (0.2) m. Six traps of each colour were placed in a completely randomized design, checked every 2 to 4 d, and re-randomized. Six replicates of this experiment were conducted from 9-26 September. Unsexed, laboratory-reared codling moth adults (n = 5,000) were released into the orchard prior to the start of the experiment.

In all experiments, the numbers of codling moths, honeybees, and muscoid flies were counted in each trap. The predominant weather patterns during these tests were clear skies with maximum daily temperatures ranging from 20 – 32 °C.

Statistical analyses. Data were transformed with square root (x + 0.01) prior to analysis. Data from experiments 1 and 2 were analyzed with one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). The September study was analyzed with a repeated measures design (ANOVA) across five dates. Means were separated in significant ANOVA’s with Fisher’s least significance difference (Analytical Software 2001).

RESULTS

The spectral reflectance pattern of delta traps differed sharply among colours (Fig. 1). The unpainted white and the painted white and cream traps were similar exhibiting > 75% reflectance at all wavelengths > 420 nm (Fig. 1). These traps had identical reflectance in the ultraviolet at 5 – 30%. Green traps had the lowest total reflectance among colours tested with a peak reflectance (ca. 20%) at 520 nm and 570 run (until 810 run). The reflectance of the orange and red traps increased rapidly at 560 nm and 600 nm and reached a plateau of ca. 55% at 620 nm and 640 nm, respectively.

Significant differences were found in the mean capture of codling moths, honeybees and flies in all but one delta trap comparison in experiments 1 and 2 (Table 1). The fewest honeybees were caught in the red, orange, and green painted delta traps in experiment 1 and in the painted white, cream, red, and green traps in experiment 2. The painted white and cream traps caught significantly fewer honeybees than the unpainted white trap in experiment 1. all but the unpainted white trap caught significantly fewer honeybees than the orange trap in experiment 2.

The vast majority of flies caught during experiments 1 and 2 were the lesser stable fly, Muscina stabulans (Fallen), and the little house fly, Fannia canicularis L., likely immigrating from the nearby dairy. Red traps caught significantly more flies than all other colours, and orange traps caught significantly more flies than the cream, white, and unpainted traps in experiment 1. Fly captures were much higher and more variable during experiment 2, though no differences were significant (Table 1).

No codling moths were caught in the unbaited traps in experiment 1. Significant differences in the catch of codling moths in experiment 2 occurred among trap colours (Table 1). The lowest mean catches were in the unpainted and white traps but these were not significantly different from the cream and red traps. Catch was significantly higher in the orange versus the white or unpainted traps. Mean moth catch in the green traps was significantly greater than in the unpainted, white or cream traps.

The green delta trap caught significantly more codling moths than the unpainted white trap (F = 42.64; df = 1, 10; P

DISCUSSION

Visual cues are known to be an important factor affecting the close range orientation of male codling moth to females or synthetic lures (Castrovillo and Carde 1980). Visual cues may also play a role in oviposition behaviour, which occurs from late afternoon to dusk (Riedl and Loher 1980). The role of colour on the orientation of male codling moths at dusk to discrete sex pheromone sources is unknown. The correlation of our traps’ reflectance data and their relative capture of codling moth suggest that traps with low reflectance at wavelengths

It is not clear from the literature whether visual detection of a trap by a moth should increase or decrease the number of individuals captured in a sex pheromone-baited trap. For example, the compound eyes of some noctuid moths have been shown to have a bimodal sensitivity to light with peaks in the UV (350 – 370 nm) and green (500 – 575 nm) regions (Agee 1973, Mitchell et al. 1989). Yet, other noctuid species respond more strongly to traps emitting low spectral reflectance in these regions (McLaughlin et al. 1975).

The significant differences found in the effectiveness of delta traps of different colours in the capture of codling moths suggest that the potential influence of colour in previous codling moth trapping studies should be reexamined. Most of the paper and plastic traps used to monitor codling moth have been cream or white (Riedl et al. 1986). No studies with codling moth have compared traps of similar geometry that differ only in colour. Plastic bucket traps with a green flat top and a white cylindrical bottom were found to have higher seasonal catches of codling moth than paper, cream colour wing traps (Vincent et al. 1990). Knodel and Agnello (1990) found that a small, orange delta trap caught nearly twice as many codling moths as either the cream colour or white wing traps in their study. Conversely, they also reported that the all-green bucket trap caught fewer moths than any other design including two designs of a green and white bucket trap. However, these differences among the bucket traps could have been due to the significant differences in the size and geometry of the various traps’ openings.

Green delta traps in our studies appeared to be the most selective and attractive colour for monitoring codling moth. Painted or unpainted white traps and cream traps all caught honeybees. We did not determine if red, orange or green traps differ in their attractiveness for codling moth. However, green and red appeared to catch somewhat fewer honeybees than orange traps, and both green and orange caught fewer flies than red traps.

The benefit derived by excluding honeybees from codling moth traps could be cancelled by an increase in the trap’s captures of large flies in some orchards. The capture of flies varied widely among our experiments due to differences in location and seasonality. The highest numbers of flies were caught in experiment 2 in an orchard situated near a dairy, and only negligible captures of flies occurred in experiment 3 in an orchard surrounded by other orchards. The higher counts of flies in experiment 2 versus experiment 1 were likely due to an increase in the mean maximum temperatures (> 3 °C) that occurred from late April to early May.

Visual stimuli are well known to be important factors affecting the behaviour of muscoid flies (McCann and Arnett 1972). The influence of colour on the level of attractiveness to the stable fly, Stomoxys calcitrans L., has been shown to be red > green > yellow > white (Muniz and Hecht 1968). The behavioural responses of S. calcitrans; the face fly, Musca autumnalis De Geer; and the horn fly, Haematobia irritons L., are all greatest to surfaces with 50%) in the range from 360 – 450 nm (Fig. 1). Thus it might be possible to reduce the capture of muscoid flies in the green, red, and orange traps if a UV reflector was added to the trap’s surface.

The congregation of muscoid flies to surfaces can also be their response to regulate their body temperature (Bushman and Patterson 1981). Flies may congregate on warmer surfaces during cool mornings or afternoons (Agee and Patterson 1983). The interior surface of the darker delta traps during the summer can be 2 – 4% warmer than white traps (unpublished data). Further research detailing the influence of temperature and other climatic factors on the capture of muscoid flies in delta traps may allow us to further improve the selectivity of this trap in monitoring codling moth. Further studies on the visual sensitivity of codling moth and its associated behaviour may allow us to develop a trap that is both more attractive for codling moth and less attractive to muscoid flies.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to thank Brad Christianson and Duane Larson, USDA, ARS, Wapato, WA for their help in setting up the field trials, and Dr. Tom Larson (Suterra, Bend, OR) for providing the traps. Jim Hansen, USDA, ARS, Wapato, WA and Rick Hilton, Oregon State University, Medford, OR provided helpful reviews. This research was partially funded by the Washington Tree Fruit Research Commission, Wenatchee, WA.

Copyright Entomological Society of British Columbia Dec 2003

Provided by ProQuest Information and Learning Company. All rights Reserved.