Beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, group sizes in Cook Inlet, Alaska, based on observer counts and aerial video

Roderick C. Hobbs

Introduction

It is extremely difficult to get accurate counts of large groups of cetaceans from aerial surveys, even though aircraft are usually better observation platforms than vessels or shore-based sites. To overcome this problem, methods and analyses for using video records were developed to obtain counts and estimate group sizes to correct observers’ aerial counts. These methods were applied to a study of beluga whales, Delphinapterus leucas, in Cook Inlet that involved annual aerial surveys conducted by NOAA’s National Marine Fisheries Service (NMFS) from 1994 to 2000 (Rugh et al., 2000).

The status of the Cook Inlet stock has been of special concern because of its small size (Hobbs et al., 2000), isolation (Laidre et al., 2000), and annual hunting pressures by Native Alaskans (Mahoney and Shelden, 2000). Studies of abundance of this stock and related parameters for correcting aerial counts and assessing variance have been conducted by NMFS annually since 1993.

During late spring and early summer, large aggregations of beluga whales are found near river mouths of upper Cook Inlet (Rugh et al., 2000). In recent years, very few whales have been observed anywhere other than in these nearshore areas, and most of the observed whales were within dense concentrations. Consequently, the accuracy of an abundance estimate for this population depends on accurate estimates of the sizes of these few large groups.

Since 1994, a consistent method has been used by the NMFS for counting groups of beluga whales during annual aerial surveys of Cook Inlet. This method adapted available survey designs (flying consistently at an altitude of 244 m (800 ft) and 185 km/h (100 knots) along coastal tracklines or straight offshore tracklines) to make systematic, thorough searches for whale groups (Rugh et al., 2000). Paired, independent observers provided sighting information used in an analysis of the likelihood that a group of whales was not seen (Hobbs et al., 2000). When a group was found, repeated counts were made using an extended racetrack flight pattern, such that observers always counted on one side of the aircraft while passing a group on a straight line. During each pass, two observers counted the group independently, and a third recorded the group on videotape. The timing of the start and stop of each count was recorded precisely along with the apparent quality of the counts–whether or not the whole group was in view or visibility interfered with the count.

These counting methods worked well for obtaining counts for each group and for comparing the performance of different observers. However, counts made during each pass varied widely among observers, and counts by each observer varied for successive counts of the same group, even when visibility did not change. Large, dense groups remained especially difficult to count accurately despite the standardized approach. Observers responded differently to increases in beluga density, with some observers continuing to count individual whales but narrowing their field of view, while other observers switched to counting in 10’s or 20’s, maintaining a larger field of view but reducing the accuracy of their counts. In addition, animals were missed due to visibility biases (availability and perception biases) (Marsh and Sinclair, 1989), which were not easily measured.

Availability bias occurs when whales are not visible to observers because the whales are completely below the surface–especially in turbid water–and so are not “available” to be counted (Marsh and Sinclair, 1989). In upper Cook Inlet, the waters are extremely turbid to the point of being nearly opaque. Secchi disk depth readings range from 1 cm to 37 cm with an average of 14 cm in areas where beluga whales are typically found (Shelden and Angliss (1)), such that any part of a beluga that is below the surface is out of sight.

Water disturbance patterns created when whales swim near the surface are often visible and can serve as sighting cues; however, observers were asked to count only those belugas that they actually saw at the surface. It is unknown to what extent their counts are influenced by the water disturbance cues. Researchers have corrected for availability bias using the formula of McLaren (1961) which uses dive interval information to estimate the inverse of the probability that a typical animal will be at the surface during the period of observation (Frost et al., 1985; Barlow et al., 1988; Laake et al., 1997). The McLaren formula also requires knowing the time that an observer spends counting whales in each patch of water which, as noted above, can vary by observer and group size.

Perception bias occurs when whales are on the surface of the water (available to be seen) but are not detected by an observer due to various possibilities, including adverse conditions (such as glare or rough seas), camouflage coloration, or observer fatigue or inexperience (Marsh and Sinclair, 1989). Perception bias is further compounded for beluga whales due to their age-specific coloration. Belugas are dark gray when born and gradually lighten as they get older, until they become completely white as adults (9-11 years) (Sergeant, 1973). When at the surface, belugas appear as white or gray ovals against a dark background of water. The white adult whales strongly contrast with the water, which makes them easy to see even at a distance. The smaller, darker whales are more likely missed in aerial counts.

To circumvent these inherent problems with observers’ counts from an aircraft, a method using video recordings was developed as an alternative. Video recordings have precise timing, have a well defined field of view, and can be examined closely frame-by-frame to ensure that all of the recorded beluga images are counted. These properties allowed for precise, repeatable counts, and accurate measurements of the time available for counting, including the time that whales were at the surface. The objectives of the video recordings during these surveys and subsequent analysis were to collect the following types of data:

1) Accurate counts of all visible surfacings in a group over a measured period of time.

2) Accurate measurement of the time whales were visible at the surface (start to end of a surfacing).

3) Apparent size of whale images in the video recordings.

4) Gray-scale classification for whale images in video recordings.

5) And, matching of whale images between simultaneous video recordings at two different magnifications.

Using these data and the average dive interval from a radio-tagging study (Lerczak et al., 2000), the counts of surfacing whales from the video could be corrected for availability bias and perception bias. Availability bias could be estimated using the McLaren (1961) formula, and perception bias could be estimated by matching images between simultaneous high and low magnification video recordings of the same group. The calculations of group size resulting from the video analysis could then be used to develop a correction method for the observer counts independent of assumptions about observer behavior and counting techniques.

Methods

Aerial Counts of Beluga Groups

Aerial counts were made by pairs of observers tallying beluga whale sightings independently during counting passes (Rugh et al., 2000). A racetrack flight pattern, typically 2-4 km from end-to-end and 1-2 km across, depending on the size of a group, usually allowed two counting passes per circuit around the group, if glare was not a problem (Fig. 1). Start and stop times were precisely noted for each counting pass, and observers graded the conditions during each count (from A to D for excellent to poor). The protocol was to continue this process until each of the observers had made at least four counts under acceptable conditions (A or B). Then observers traded positions, and the process was repeated.

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

Typically, then, each group of whales was counted 16 times (4 times by 4 different observers). This method allowed for repeatable, independent counts by four observers with essentially the same presentation of the group and a measure of time spent counting. The number of counts was reduced when groups were small (<5 whales) or dense air traffic prevented staying in the area. Each observer recorded counts independently, and counts were not discussed among observers during the remainder of the project. To further maintain independence, the counts were entered into the database by a colleague who did not participate in the counts, or they were entered only after the survey was completed.

Standardized Counting Video

Videotape recordings (hereafter referred to as counting video), concurrent to observers’ counts of beluga whale groups, were collected with a Canon (2) Hi-8 814 XL-S video camera during 1994-98 and a Sony Digital 8 DCRTRV103 video camera during 1999-2000. These tapes were used to count whales in each group independent of the observers. In the aircraft, the videographer was positioned at an open window facing the same direction as the two observers so that all three would have essentially the same view of each group of whales (Rugh et al., 2000 provides details).

Videotape, with a time-stamp, was recorded during nearly every counting pass over a beluga group. The counting video was taken in either of two ways: 1) The “point” method was used if the whole group could be seen in the viewfinder of the video camera. In this case, the camera was pointed at the group and moved to keep the entire group of whales in the field of view until they were out of sight (Fig. 2a); or 2) The “scanned” method was used when groups were too dispersed for all individuals to be in view at one time. With this method, the camera was held still and perpendicular to the trackline so that the video scanned the length of the group as the aircraft moved past (Fig. 2b).

[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]

Zoomed Video

Although the counting video was valuable for counting clearly visible, distinct whales in a group, small whales may not have been visible in the video at the limits of the resolution, or whales may not have been distinguishable due to close proximity with other whales. In 1996, a second video camera (Ricoh Hi-8 R800H) was used at a higher magnification (8x) during counting passes, to test the visibility of small and gray whales in the standard video.

Upon examination of the zoomed video, we found that the narrow field of view resulted in capturing few usable whale images during these passes. An experiment was then devised in which a large group of beluga whales was circled continuously while both cameras videotaped the group. The two video cameras (standard and zoomed) were mounted side by side on a board so they were held steady and parallel to each other. The circling allowed whales to be in view for longer periods of time and, thus, they were more likely to be captured on the zoomed video. This circling experiment was repeated in 1998 and 1999.

In June 2000, a Sony Digital Camcorder DSR-PD 100A replaced the Ricoh as the zoomed video camera, and it was used during counting passes instead of a circling experiment. It had higher resolution and could be zoomed to a lower magnification (~5x) and still collect useable whale images while allowing a broader field of view. Sufficient zoomed images were captured using this camera during counting passes so that a separate experiment was not necessary.

It was assumed that within the viewing range of the zoomed video, all whales at the surface could be distinguished. We believe this assumption is reasonable, because the higher magnification allowed positive identification of small dark gray whales next to large white whales and the distinction between larger gray and white animals.

Video Analysis

Each pass on the videotapes was reviewed to evaluate its quality for counting beluga whale groups and was given a rating (excellent, good, fair, poor, or unacceptable). The highest rating was given to passes in which the camera panned smoothly while remaining in focus and the margins of a group were visible. Video passes were given a lower rating if all or a part of a group was obscured by glare, confused by waves, or if focus, magnification, or panning varied rapidly. Only excellent and good passes were used for the group size estimation analysis.

The video recordings were examined using a Panasonic high resolution monitor and a Hi-8 video cassette recorder (VCR) (Sony EVO 9500A) (1994-98) or the Sony digital camera in playback mode (1999-2000) capable of advancing and reversing the tape frame-by-frame and assigning a unique time and frame count number to each frame. Each frame corresponded to 1/30th of a second.

Groups were counted in two different ways according to group size: 1) for small groups, whales were counted directly from the monitor screen as the video played at regular or slow speed and 2) for large groups, whale locations were “captured” by stopping the video every 0.5 seconds (15 frames). This timing was based on the assumption that no whale would surface and dive again within a 0.5-sec period.

Transparency sheets were placed on the monitor screen and marked with dots to indicate the position of each beluga image. The sheets were then compared by placing one on top of the next; this allowed for differentiating between new sightings and whales which were resighted from one sheet to the next. New sightings were marked as new and tallied by sheet. The number of whales on the first sheet plus the number of “new” whales on each successive sheet were then summed to derive a total count for the aerial pass.

To correct for availability bias, the time spent counting was needed. This was determined differently depending on how the video was taken. For the “point” groups, the amount of time that the group was in view was used. For the “scanned” groups, timing was based on how long an object on the water was in view across the screen.

This method of counting beluga whales from video recordings was developed in several steps. Initially, two people viewed an aerial pass together with open discussion about which images represented actual beluga whales. Then a second series of counts were made by three people independently (including the two who had made the first count together). These counts were then examined sheet-by-sheet (every 0.5-sec) by all three reviewers. Discrepancies between reviewers were discussed, and a consensus was reached on whether a dot on the screen could be considered a whale or not.

For example, we found that a white bird in the video could be mistaken for a beluga whale, but we learned through this process how to recognize which dots were birds. After the method was developed by the initial group of three analysts, new analysts were trained by following a progression from open discussion, checks on their counts by experienced analysts, to independent analysis.

Time-at-the-surface, Image Size, and Gray-scale

To determine the distribution of whale image sizes visible from the aerial video and to estimate the time that whales were visible at the surface, a sample of whales was examined from each of the passes that were counted on transparency sheets. For each group, whales from every tenth sheet (every 5 sec) were identified with a number. Since the average dive interval is typically longer than 20 sec (Lerczak et al., 2000), this method essentially grabbed a random subsample of 10-30 whales per group. Each numbered whale was then followed using the frame-by-frame mode on the video cassette recorder; essentially following a whale from the time it first appeared to the time it disappeared. Time and frame numbers were used to determine the length of time each whale was visible at the surface, so that the error in timing was at most 0.07 sec.

Images of whales that were measured for time-at-the-surface were also measured for image size and given a gray-scale rating. Measurements were taken at the halfway point in each whale’s time-at-the-surface. A plastic metric ellipse template was used as a scale for size. The template was copied, doubled, and reduced by half (using a photocopier) to create a set of standard sizes of white ellipses on a gray background to match the typical image and range of sizes of the whale images. Each whale ellipse was classified with the length of the major axis (in millimeters) relative to the standardized scaled chart. Measurements by two independent analysts were averaged. A gray-scale rating for each whale (bright white, dull white, and gray) was determined by consensus.

Comparison of Counting Video and Zoomed Video

The zoomed video was evaluated for usable sections. Using two video players and monitors side-by-side, segments from the zoomed video were matched to segments taken from the counting video by comparing the spatial and movement patterns of whales. The two videotapes were synchronized to the nearest frame possible. Once segments were synchronized, the relative magnification was measured by finding a pair of whales on the zoomed video that could be matched on the standard video.

Positions of the whales were marked on transparencies sheets placed on the monitor screens and distances in millimeters measured between the pair of whales on each screen. Using the position of the matched whales and the relative magnification, the boundary of the zoomed image on the counting video could be calculated. A rectangle was then drawn on the standard video sheet, marking the boundaries of the zoomed image. Marks were made on the transparencies indicating whether whales were visible on both videotapes or only seen on the zoomed video.

The size of whale images in the zoomed frames were then measured following the method of the counting video (using the half-way time calculated for the counting video when it was available) and assigned a gray-scale rating following a scheme with four shades (white, off-white, light gray, and dark gray). Because the images were larger, it was possible to distinguish four gray-shades rather than the three shades used in the standard video.

Correction for Perception Bias

Detecting whales in video recordings is limited by the resolution of the video system. Probability of detection was measured by comparing the whales seen in the zoomed video to those seen in the corresponding region on the counting video. The whale images in the zoomed video were each assigned to one of three categories: 1) whales that were seen in both the zoomed and counting video, 2) whales in the zoomed video that were missed in the counting video due to proximity–two whales surfacing close to each other appear as one large image on the counting video, or 3) image size–a whale seen in the zoomed is too small or gray so that it falls below a threshold and does not form a visible image on the counting video. The two mechanisms (proximity and image size) that affect whale detection in the counting video require different approaches for correction.

Proximity Correction

When two whales were close enough together to appear as a single whale on the counting video, the space between them was much narrower than the width of an average whale. Consequently, these two images would be merged throughout the typical range of magnifications used in the counting video, regardless of their relative size. Thus, a constant ratio could be used to correct for whales missed due to proximity:

[J.sub.z]/[[J.sub.z]-[J.sub.p]],

where [J.sub.z] is the number of whales seen in the zoomed video and [J.sub.p] is the number of whales missed due to proximity.

Image Size Correction

The resolution of a video system is limited by the density of scan lines in the video recording system and the density of pixels on the display monitor. This process of scanning and pixelation has the effect of smearing images and edges by averaging the gray scale and hue across each pixel. If a pixel is half water and half beluga, then it will appear to have a gray scale and hue halfway between that of the water and the beluga. A large, white beluga will appear as a bright white ellipse with a fuzzy edge that fades to the gray-scale of the water. A gray beluga will appear as a gray ellipse with a less distinct fading to the water color. Small, gray belugas or distant belugas of any hue may not have a sufficiently large image to completely fill any pixels so that the image is entirely made up of these averaged pixels.

Because the edge of the image has been blurred, it is necessary to interpret by eye the margin of the image from the surrounding background. Experience has shown that with a limited amount of training, consistent and repeatable measurements of beluga images can be made. However, the measurement method is partially subjective, so it is necessary to estimate the bias in the interpreted image size. The smearing occurs only at the edges, so the bias should be independent of size. The gradient that is interpreted is dependent on the difference in hue between the object and the background. The subjectivity involves a determination of the point along this gradient that is the edge of the image.

The lengths of the images at the mid times of beluga surfacings matched between the counting and zoomed videos can be related by the following formula:

[L.sub.z]/m = [[l.sub.z] – b]/m = [l.sub.c] – b = [L.sub.c],

where [L.sub.z] and [L.sub.c] are the unbiased sizes of the whale images on the zoomed and counting videos, respectively; [l.sub.z] and [l.sub.c] are the measured sizes of the whale images on the zoomed and counting videos, respectively; m, is the relative magnification between the zoomed and counting video frames (obtained as the ratio of the distance between centers of two whale images seen on both the counting and zoomed video); and b is the bias resulting from smearing of the edge.

An average value for the bias can be estimated from several image pairs as,

[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII]

where [J.sub.n] is the number of whales seen in both the zoomed and counting video, and j is the index of the jth pair. If b was not significantly different from zero, it was not necessary to correct for bias.

The following equation was then used to estimate the image size in the counting video for the whales that were visible in the zoomed video but, because of their size, were not detected in the counting video. The estimated image size for these whales in the counting video was:

[l.sub.c] = [[[l.sub.z] – b]/m] + b.

A binomial logistic regression was applied to the resulting combined distribution of measured and estimated standard image sizes to estimate the probability that a whale with a given image size would be seen in the counting video.

For a given group, g, and pass, p, m is not known. Instead, the average of image sizes, [[mu].sub.g,p] and the fractions of whales that would be detected, F([[mu].sub.g,p]), in a counting video are related. To determine this relationship, arbitrary values for magnification, m’, (e.g. magnification increasing at 0.01 intervals) are chosen to span the range of possible magnifications. The combined distribution of observed (whales seen in both the zoomed and counting video) and estimated (whales seen in the zoomed but missed in the counting because of small size) counting video sizes are then rescaled by

[l.sub.m’j] = ([l.sub.cj] – b)m’ + b

to simulate the distribution of image sizes under these arbitrary magnifications. For this re-scaled distribution, the average of image sizes, [mu](m’), and the fractions of whales, F([mu](m’)), that would be detected in a counting video are

[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII]

where P(l) is the probability that an image of size I will be seen in the counting video.

A lookup table relating average image size for a group counted from video, [[mu].sub.g,p], to the correction for the fraction that were missed because of image size, 1/F([[mu]g,p]), was created from this analysis. For passes with a sample of measured images, the fraction missed was found in the table. Passes of small groups where images were not measured were given the average fraction missed from other passes of the same group, or if no other passes on the group had measured images, the pass was given the average fraction missed of all measured passes from all groups.

Combined Correction Factor

The correction for perception bias was the product of the proximity correction and the image size correction. For a video count with an estimated average image size, [[mu].sub.g,p], the correction factor, [D.sub.g,p] is then,

[D.sub.gp] = [[J.sub.z]/[[J.sub.z] – [J.sub.p]]]1/F([[mu].sub.gp]).

Correction for Availability

The formula of McLaren (1961) for the correction for availability bias is the inverse of the probability that a typical beluga is at or will appear at the surface during the videotaping. The correction factor, [A.sub.g,p], for a group and pass depending on the time spent counting, [t.sub.g,p], is calculated as,

[A.sub.g,p] = [T.sub.1]/[[T.sub.s] + [t.sub.g,p]],

where [T.sub.I] is the average dive interval (24.1 sec., Lerczak et al., 2000), and [T.sub.s] is the average time at the surface from the video analysis described above.

Estimation of Group Size

The group size, [n.sub.g], was estimated by averaging the corrected video counts for a group:

[n.sub.g] = 1/[P.sub.g][summation over [P.sub.g]] [c.sub.g,p][D.sub.g,p][A.sub.g,p],

where, [c.sub.g,p] is the count for group g from pass p, and [P.sub.g] as the number of passes for group g that were counted. When a video pass contained two or more distinctly different segments (e.g. it began using the point method, then switched to the scan method when the first portion of the group came abeam of the plane), the counts were corrected separately to create a group size estimate for each subgroup. These subgroup estimates were then summed to estimate the total group size.

The coefficient of variation (CV) for [n.sub.g] was estimated as:

CV([n.sub.g]) = [square root of [bar]C[V.sup.2](n)/[P.sub.g] + [bar]C[V.sup.2]([D.sub.g]) + [bar]C[V.sup.2]([A.sub.g])].

An average CV for a group size estimate made from a single count was estimated by averaging the variation of the group size estimates of all groups where more than one pass from the group was counted from video,

[bar]C[V.sup.2](n) = [summation over G2] 1/[[P.sub.g] – 1] [summation over [P.sub.g]] [([n.sub.g,p] – [[bar]n.sub.g]).sup.2]/[summation over G2][[bar]n.sup.2.sub.g].

Where more than one count is used to estimate group size, this average CV is scaled appropriately. The value CV(n) includes an empirical measure of stochastic variation between counts that is not corrected by the two correction factors, but it does not account for the variation of the correction factors themselves which must be accounted for separately.

The component of the CV resulting from the correction for perception, CV([D.sub.g]), is estimated by the delta method as,

[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII]

For cases where [[mu].sub.g,p] was not estimated, the correction factor [D.sub.g,p] was derived from an average of [[mu].sub.g,p] from other passes of the same group or an average of other groups. In these cases SE([[mu]g,p]) was the standard deviation of the set of the estimated average image sizes of the averaged groups.

The component of the CV resulting from the correction for availability, CV([D.sub.g]), is dominated by the variation of [T.sub.I]. The variation of [T.sub.I] has a component related to the variability between individuals and the variation of a typical individual. Following the delta method yields,

[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII]

where [[sigma].sup.2.sub.A] (= 41 [sec.sup.2], [[sigma].sub.A]) = 6.4 sec) and [[sigma].sup.2.sub.I] (= 707 [sec.sup.2], [[sigma].sub.I] = 26.6 sec) are the variance of the average dive interval among individuals and the average variance of the dive interval of individuals, respectively (values taken from Lerczak et al., 2000). Note that in this formulation, CV(A) was not independent of group size because of the assumption that the dive behavior of individuals in the group is uncorrelated so that the variation in the average of dive intervals during the counting interval decreases as group size increases.

Group Size Estimates from Observer Counts

Good quality video was not available for all groups, so a method for estimating group size from observer counts was devised. Aerial counts of beluga whales were corrected for observer differences and the effect of encounter rate (group density in whales per second). Data from observers who had participated in the equivalent of one or more complete survey seasons (three surveys of the upper inlet and one survey of the lower inlet) were included in the analysis. Only counts made during passes considered by the observers to be excellent or good in quality (A or B) were used. Group sizes, estimated from video recordings, were used to represent the true group size.

This method provided a correction for availability and perception as well as the uncertainty in the time available to observers to count individual whales. The correction formula was derived by regression of the video-derived group sizes against the observer counts for those groups and an interaction term between the counts and the observed encounter rate with the intercept fixed at zero:

[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII]

where [n.sub.g,p,o] is the size estimate for group g from a count by observer o during pass p, [b.sub.1o], [b.sub.2o] are the parameters estimated for each observer by linear regression, [C.sub.g,p,o] is the count by observer o of group g during pass p, [t.sub.g,p] is the time spent counting group g during pass p, S[E.sup.2](b) is the squared standard error of the recession coefficients, [b.sub.1,o], [b.sub.2,o], and Cov ([b.sub.1,o], [b.sub.2,o]) is the estimated covariance of the regression coefficients, [b.sub.1,o], [b.sub.2,o].

This approach weights the correction formula to be most accurate for large groups where a bias would have the greatest impact on the abundance estimate. The first summand estimates a multiplicative correction for counts to group size; the second summand estimates an additive bias proportional to the count multiplied by the density of the group. For aerial counts without recorded time, a single multiplicative correction was also estimated. The correction formula was applied to counts from groups where no group size estimate was available through the video analysis. These corrected counts were then averaged to estimate the group size:

[MATHEMATICAL EXPRESSION NOT REPRODUCIBLE IN ASCII]

where [n.sub.g] is the estimated size of group g, and [J.sub.g] is the set of corrected observer counts for group g.

Results

Aerial Counts

There were 144 sightings of beluga whale groups during aerial surveys of Cook Inlet in 1994-2000 (Table 1). Many of these represent multiple resightings of groups encountered on different survey days and years. Of the 144 sightings, 126 were counted and video recorded following the survey protocol. The remaining were either: 1) small groups (<10) encountered in areas of high aircraft traffic, where groups could not be circled due to safety concerns, or 2) groups encountered during non-survey flights (e.g. in support of vessel operations or as a part of adjunct experiments). Video of sufficient quality for group size estimates was available for 79 groups, averaging 3.5 (SD=2.1) counts per group. Average count times ranged from 4 sec to 13 sec for scan counts and from 6 sec to 65 sec for point counts. The remaining 65 group size estimates were derived from observer counts, averaging 4.7 (SD=3.2) counts per group with count times ranging from 11 sec to 115 sec.

Linear regression, comparing observer counts to counts from video for the years 1994-98, indicated significant effects for each of the covariates: count ([b.sub.1,o]) and count multiplied by encounter rate ([b.sub.2,o]). All but one of the individual observer parameters for the count correction were significant, and for three observers, the parameter in the adjustment for encounter rate was significantly different from zero (Table 2). The video recordings in 1999 and 2000 were so successful that sizes for all but a few small groups were obtained from video. Sample sizes for comparison between the video-derived group sizes and observer counts within each year were sufficiently large so that each year was treated separately in the linear regression analysis (Table 2). Group sizes were then estimated for groups which had no group size available from video.

Video Counts, Time-at-the-surface, and Image Size

A total of 275 passes representing 79 groups were counted using aerial video recordings and a total of 2,775 whales were measured in the counting video. The average time-at-the-surface for all of the whales that were measured was 2.34 sec (SD=0.94, SE=0.018). This value, rounded to the nearest 0.5 sec ([T.sub.s] = 2.5 sec), was used in the correction for whales missed under the water’s surface. The average image size was 1.76 mm (SD=0.46, SE=0.009).

Zoomed vs. Counting Video and Correction for Whales Missed at the Surface

Two of the three circling experiments provided enough zoomed images suitable for matching to the counting video. A whale group videotaped 13 June 1996 resulted in 103 usable whale images in the zoomed video, of which 91 were also seen in the counting video, 9 were not seen, and 3 were missed due to proximity to another whale that was counted. A group videotaped in 15 June 1998 resulted in 231 usable whale images in the zoomed video, of which 192 were also seen in the counting video, 29 were not seen, and 10 were missed due to proximity. The average whale image size between years was not significantly different (1996: 1.92 mm, SE=0.033 mm; 1998: 1.90 mm, SE=0.023), so the two data sets were combined into one size distribution.

Logistic regression estimated the probability that a whale image of a given size was seen as:

P(l) = [e.sup.-13.72+9.56l]/[1 + [e.sup.-13.72+9.56l]].

Thus an image of 1.43 mm in size would have a 50% chance of being seen in the counting video (Fig. 3). There was a poor correspondence between the gray-scale codes given to the images in the zoomed and counting videos. Gray belugas always appeared gray, but white belugas in the zoomed video would sometimes appear gray in the counting video when their image size was small. Therefore, gray-scale codes were not used to stratify this analysis, and the ratio of gray to white images in the counting video is not considered to be representative of the ratio of juveniles to adults in the population. The correction for missed whales at the surface, by average image size is fairly constant for larger values ([mu] > 3 mm) but climbs quickly as the average image size declines below 2.5 mm (Fig. 4). This correction was not considered valid for values greater than 1.5 ([mu] < 1.63 mm); however, there were no passes counted from videotape when average sizes were below this cutoff.

[FIGURES 3-4 OMITTED]

For groups counted from video using the Canon Hi-8 video camera (1994-98) this correction averaged 1.17 (SD=0.04) and ranged from 1.10 to 1.35 (Table 1). The component of this correction that corrected for whales missed due to image size averaged 1.13 (SD=0.04) and ranged from 1.06 to 1.30. The component related to whales missed due to proximity in the counting video was 1.04 (CV=0.01).

Zoomed video of groups during counting passes in 2000 was of sufficient quality for comparison to the counting video (with the Sony Digital 8 camera). A total of 24 passes on 11 groups resulted in 170 usable whale images in the zoomed video; of these, 154 were also seen in the counting video, 13 were not seen, and 3 were missed due to proximity to another whale that was counted. Accordingly, a logistic regression estimate of the probability that a whale image of a given size was seen was:

P(l) = [e.sup.-5.77+7.00l]/[1 + [e.sup.-5.77+7.00l]].

Thus an image of 0.834 mm in size would have a 50% chance of being seen in the counting video (Fig. 3b). Gray scale codes did not correspond sufficiently well between the images in the zoomed and counting videos and therefore were not used to stratify this analysis.

The correction for missed whales at the surface, by average image size is fairly constant for larger values ([mu] > 2 mm) but climbs quickly as the average image size declines below 1.5 mm (Fig. 4). This correction was not considered valid for values greater than 1.5 ([mu] < 1.10 mm); however, there were no passes counted from videotape where average sizes fell below this cutoff. For groups counted from video using the Sony Digital 8 video camera (1999-2000), this correction averaged 1.17 (SD=0.08) and ranged from 1.06 to 1.34 (Table 1). The component of this correction that corrected for whales missed due to image size averaged 1.15 (SD=0.08) and ranged from 1.04 to 1.31. The component related to whales missed due to proximity in the counting video was 1.018 (CV=0.01).

Correction for Whales Missed Below the Surface in Video

Of the 275 passes counted from video, 30 were point counts, 189 were scan counts, and 56 were a mixture of scan and point. Point counts averaged 18.4 sec and ranged from 6 sec to 66 sec; scan counts averaged 9.7 sec and ranged from 4 sec to 30 sec. Correction factors for individual passes averaged 2.03 (SD=0.64) and ranged from 0.88 to 3.62 per whale at the surface.

Group Size Estimates

Group size estimates ranged from 1 to 731 (Table 1). CV’s for group size estimates from video averaged 0.36 and ranged from 0.09 to 1.10. CV’s for group size estimates from observer counts averaged 0.45 and ranged from 0.11 to u 1.15. The largest component of the CV’s of the group size estimates from video is CV(A), the correction for availability, averaging 0.28, which is largely determined by the uncertainty in the dive interval.

Discussion

Video-derived group size estimates proved to be a valuable tool for examining observer counts made from an aircraft. When confronted with large groups, it was difficult for observers to quickly see and count each individual whale. Therefore, observers made quick estimates by tallying whales as best as they could.

Data from video counts indicate that for these large groups, too many whales were present for observers to mentally register each whale. Often, biased counts were made depending on the response of individual observers to this difficulty. The observer counts of relatively small groups (< 50 whales) were often larger than counts from the video but not larger than the corrected video group size estimates, suggesting that observers had more time to count individuals in small groups, and they could search a larger area than the video could record at any one time while scanning.

In making corrections for missed whales in the video analysis, it was necessary to have a measure of the portion of a group of whales that were actually visible in the video. Of the two ways that whales could be missed, it was far more likely that they were unavailable to be videotaped due to submersion than undetectable due to the resolution of the video camera.

The average correction factor for availability bias was 2.03 (SD=0.64), thus more than half of the whales in a typical group were missed because they were underwater, and in some groups more than 2/3 of the group were missed. By comparison, the average correction for whales missed at the surface was 1.17, or roughly 1/7 of the whales available to be recorded were missed. On the surface it would seem that a higher resolution video system would reduce these two corrections by providing: 1) a wider field of view, which would allow for longer scan times and more whales to surface in the view, and 2) a crisper image so that small whales would be easier to detect, and whales in close proximity would form more distinct images. Such a system was incorporated in the aerial survey program in 1999 and 2000 when it started using a digital video camera. This resulted in an improved resolution of over 40% when comparing the estimated probabilities by size in Figure 3, but it did not significantly change the average correction factors. What did change was the success rate of the video data collection, resulting in 84% of the group size estimates being made from video in 1999 and 2000, whereas 46% of estimates were made from video during 1994-98.

The measure of time-at-the-surface was necessary as a component in the correction factor for whales missed because they were underwater during the count. We found differences in surface times between whale groups, although two passes from the same group had similar surface times. There are several possible reasons for these differences. The behavior of the animals may have been different between groups (or in the same group on different days). Whales may have faster surfacing times when they are active (feeding or traveling) than when at rest, or they may have different surfacing times while feeding on different prey types (such as herding small fish in dense schools compared to targeting large, single fish). However, we were unable to determine the behavior of a whale group from the air, other than that they were clumped or spread out.

Surface times may also be different between groups due to differences in distance from the aircraft. The farther the group was from the aircraft, the smaller the whales appeared. They would, therefore, be less distinct when measuring surface times. The brightness of the day and the sea condition may also affect how well animals were seen. These weather conditions affect how whales contrast against the background water gray-scale. There could also have been a difference in the brightness settings of the video monitor during analysis. However, variation of the average surface times of passes and groups was small in comparison to the uncertainty in the dive interval and the half-second resolution of the counting time, so it was not necessary to estimate this separately for each pass.

The variability of dive intervals of individuals is problematic for variance calculations. Individual behavior may be correlated if all or most of the members of a group are engaged in the same activity; this component of the variance could remain constant regardless of group size. An example would be a group that was feeding and consequently taking longer dives; each individual would be diving for a longer time within its own range of variability. The variance of the average dive interval among individuals would still decline with the inverse of group size. It is unknown to what degree the dive behavior of individuals in a group is correlated. We do not see obvious patterns during aerial surveys, so we assume that–although small subgroups may have correlated behavior–the overall effect is negligible.

The gray-scale codes that were determined for the counting video did not correspond well to the codes given to the corresponding zoomed images. Zoomed images were a better record of the tree shade of the whale, so the lack of good correspondence to the gray-scale codes of the counting video indicated that the counting video was not a useful record of the ratio of gray to white in a group of beluga whales (i.e. the ratio of juveniles to adults). The most likely reason is that sizes of whale images in the counting video are close to the limits of the resolution of the video system. Higher resolution in the new digital video system made these gray-scale codes more reliable, but in the size range where whales were most likely to be missed, there was little improvement.

Despite conscientious effort by observers to count as many whales as they could, it is apparent from the magnitude of the correction for each observer’s group size estimates that a significant number of beluga whales were missed during aerial counts even among well-trained observers. The magnitude of this correction is considerably larger with untrained observers. The most likely explanation is that each observer can only effectively count in an area somewhat smaller than the area covered by a large group. The time spent counting any portion of the group was then only an unknown fraction of the time spent counting the whole group.

The McLaren formula was not appropriate for use with these data because the time available to count an individual whale was not well determined (except in the video analysis). Whale groups were often spread over several kilometers of water, so the time spent counting was sometimes as much as a minute or more, but no area of water was in view for more than several seconds. Also, observers have noticed that when whale density was high, the effective field of view for counting was reduced, decreasing the time available to count each individual whale.

Using video analysis to develop correction factors resolved this problem because there was no limit to the amount of time available to study the image. A correction specific to each observer allowed for variation in the area that each observer searched during an aerial count and the different responses of individual observers to high densities of whales.

A stranding of a group of beluga whales on a mud flat during low tide on 12 June 1996 allowed a rare opportunity to compare a precise count from aerial video recording of the group when stranded (63) against the typical, systematic aerial observer and video counts (racetrack method) made after the tide returned, and the whales were able to swim away. The resultant corrected count from aerial video of the swimming whales (69; Table 1, group 3) compares well to the precise count (63) obtained from the video of the group when stranded. The average of observer counts for this group (31) after it was waterborne again provides an anecdotal correction factor estimate of 2.2 (= 69/31), which is consistent with the correction factors for observer counts obtained from the video passes used in the abundance estimates (Table 2).

Other authors have applied correction factors to calculate beluga abundance from aerial counts. Brodie (1971) used a factor of 1.4 based on observed diving behavior (not including hard-to-see juvenile whales). Sergeant (1973) estimated that belugas in fairly turbid water would be visible for about one-third of the time (i.e. a factor of 3), based on observations of whales one day at the mouth of the Churchill Riven Fraker (1980) used a factor of 2 to account for belugas invisible beneath the surface, but he considered this “largely arbitrary.” Kingsley (1998), in a review of surveys of belugas in the St. Lawrence River, used a minimum availability correction of 1.15 for whales that were not at the surface in aerial photographs, but this was to improve the conformity with visual estimates, not a correction for absolute abundance.

The most substantial correction factor, 2.75, was developed by Frost et al. (1985) through results from VHF tags kept 2 weeks on two beluga whales in Bristol Bay and the assumption that aerial observers have 10 sec to search an area. However, their technique was designed to correct for whales missed in small groups while making a single transect pass through a sample area.

Although the Cook Inlet surveys have continued to operate on straight flightlines when counting whales, the multiple fly-overs (“racetracks”) allow ample time to determine the extent of each group. This provides a better counting situation as observers can concentrate their search on the known location of the respective group. The amount of time spent counting each group–typically 20-60 sec–is recorded and can be included in calculations of abundance.

In conclusion, use of video to count groups of beluga whales removes some of the uncertainty associated with aerial observer counts. The area of view is well defined, and the time spent counting an area can be precisely measured. Little subjectivity is involved in interpreting the images, and counts from video recordings are highly repeatable among trained video analysts. Associated variables necessary for correction factors, such as time-at-the-surface and average image size, can be easily measured. Yet, the average coefficient of variation for a single count is over 20%. A portion of this is the result of the binomial variation associated with the correction factors for missed animals; the remainder may be due to variations in group behavior that we are as yet unable to identify from the air.

Table 1.–Beluga whale group size estimates made from aerial surveys

of Cook Inlet, Alaska, 1994-2000. Count method refers to either group

size estimates made from video counts only (Vid.) or from corrected

observer counts (Obs.). Elapsed times are: video point passes (P),

video scan passes (S), observer counts (O), or elapsed time not

available (na). Correction factors for group size estimates made

from video counts were calculated for each pass separately with group

averages given here. For groups with no usable video, group size was

estimated from observer counts using a formula derived by regression

of counts vs. group size where video was available (see Table 2 for

parameters).

Average Number

Count group of

Date Group Location method count counts

6/1/94 1 Big Susitna Vid. 156 4

6/1/94 2 Big Susitna Vid. 145 1

6/2/94 1 W of Big Susitna Obs. 119 4

6/2/94 2 Turnagain Arm Obs. 6 4

6/2/94 3 Chickaloon Bay Obs. 15 4

6/3/94 1 Pt Possession Obs. 10 5

6/3/94 2 Kachemak Bay Obs. 3 5

6/3/94 3 Kachemak Bay Obs. 5 3

6/4/94 1 Iniskin Bay Obs. 2 4

6/4/94 2 W of Big Susitna Obs. 59 4

6/4/94 3 Big Susitna Vid. 155 2

6/4/94 4 W of Little Susitna Vid. 123 1

6/5/94 1 Pt Possession/E Obs. 1 3

Foreland

6/5/94 2 Beluga R Vid. 16 1

6/5/94 3 W of Big Susitna Vid. 16 1

6/5/94 4 W of Big Susitna Obs. 9 6

6/5/94 5 W of Big Susitna Vid. 29 1

6/5/94 6 Little Susitna Obs. 145 6

6/5/94 7 Chickaloon Vid. 6 1

7/18/95 1 Chickaloon Vid. 18 5

7/18/95 2 McArthur R Obs. 1 8

7/18/95 3 Big Susitna Obs. 331 7

7/19/95 1 Chickaloon Vid. 14 6

7/19/95 2 McArthur R Obs. 4 5

7/19/95 3 Shirleyville Obs. 4 1

7/19/95 4 Big Susitna Vid. 101 2

7/20/95 1 Chickaloon Vid. 7 4

7/20/95 2 Big Susitna Vid. 48 1

7/20/95 3 Big Susitna Obs. 104 5

7/21/95 1 Big Susitna (E) Vid. 18 1

7/21/95 2 Big Susitna (W) Obs. 132 5

7/21/95 3 Knik Arm Obs. 2 1

7/21/95 4 Chickaloon Obs. 18 8

7/22/95 1 Big R Vid. 9 4

7/24/95 1 Drift R Obs. 2 5

7/24/95 2 McArthur R Obs. 2 12

7/24/95 3 Big Susitna (W) Vid. 86 1

7/24/95 4 Big Susitna (E) Obs. 34 7

6/11/96 1 S of Beluga R Obs. 2 1

6/11/96 2 Lewis R Obs. 6 6

6/11/96 3 Ivan R Obs. 2 1

6/11/96 4 Theodore R Obs. 4 3

6/11/96 5 Lewis R Obs. 117 13

6/12/96 1 Knik Arm Obs. 5 5

6/12/96 2 Knik Arm Obs. 2 1

6/12/96 3 Big Susitna (after Vid. 43 2

stranding)

6/12/96 4 Pt Possession Obs. 17 1

6/12/96 5 Lewis R Obs. 136 12

6/12/96 6 Theodore R Obs. 17 9

6/12/96 7 Lewis R Vid. 32 2

6/12/96 8 Big Susitna R Obs. 73 2

6/12/96 9 Big Susitna R Obs. 20 5

6/13/96 1 Knik Arm Obs. 7 7

6/13/96 2 Knik Arm Obs. 8 7

6/13/96 3 Pt Possession Vid. 26 3

6/13/96 4 Ivan R Obs. 80 8

6/13/96 5 Big Susitna R Vid. 79 3

6/14/96 1 Pt MacKenzie Obs. 18 6

6/16/96 1 Knik Arm Obs. 17 6

6/16/96 2 Knik Arm Obs. 13 10

6/16/96 3 Pt Possession Vid. 15 2

6/16/96 4 Lewis/Ivan R Vid. 107 8

6/16/96 5 Big Susitna Vid. 34 4

6/16/96 6 Big/Little Susitna Obs. 55 8

6/16/96 7 Little Susitna Vid. 21 2

6/17/96 1 Ivan/Big Susitna Vid. 125 2

6/17/96 3 Little Susitna Vid. 22 3

6/17/96 4 Ivan/Big Susitna Vid. 65 1

6/8/97 1 Knik Arm Vid. 14 3

6/8/97 2 Knik Arm Vid. 35 5

6/8/97 3 Knik Arm Vid. 42 5

6/8/97 4 Knik Arm Obs. 1 1

6/8/97 5 Knik Arm Vid. 35 5

6/8/97 7 Knik Arm Obs. 4 1

6/8/97 8 Chickaloon Vid. 11 6

6/8/97 9 Chickaloon Vid. 9 4

6/8/97 10 Big Susitna Vid. 49 3

6/9/97 1 Tuxedni Bay Obs. 2 1

6/9/97 2 Big Susitna Obs. 58 9

6/10/97 1 Chickaloon Vid. 30 4

6/10/97 2 Big Susitna Obs. 70 15

6/10/97 3 Knik Arm Vid. 67 7

6/10/97 4 Knik Arm Vid. 30 5

6/10/97 5 Knik Arm Obs. 1 1

6/10/97 6 Knik Arm Obs. 5 2

6/9/98 1 Little Susitna Vid. 59 1

6/10/98 1 Fire I Obs. 11 8

6/10/98 2 Chickaloon Vid. 14 5

6/10/98 3 Susitna Vid. 75 3

6/10/98 4 Knik Obs. 4 1

6/10/98 5 Knik Obs. 11 4

6/10/98 6 Knik Obs. 4 4

6/10/98 7 Knik Obs. 4 5

6/10/98 8 Knik Obs. 30 3

6/10/98 9 Knik Obs. 6 2

6/10/98 10 Knik Obs. 24 5

6/12/98 1 Little Susitna Vid. 26 4

6/12/98 2 Knik Obs. 5 5

6/12/98 3 Knik Vid. 14 2

6/12/98 4 Knik Vid. 10 3

6/12/98 5 Knik Vid. 8 3

6/12/98 6 Knik Obs. 9 4

6/12/98 7 Knik Vid. 19 1

6/12/98 8 Chickaloon Vid. 16 3

6/12/98 9 Chickaloon Vid. 14 2

6/15/98 1 Chickaloon Vid. 34 3

6/15/98 2 Little Susitna Obs. 2 1

6/15/98 3 Little Susitna Vid. 132 4

6/15/98 4 Knik Obs. 12 3

6/15/98 5 Knik Vid. 16 7

6/15/98 6 Knik Vid. 2 2

6/15/98 7 Knik Vid. 5 5

6/9/99 1 Little Susitna Vid. 123 6

6/9/99 2 Knik Arm Obs. 23 3

6/9/99 3 Chickaloon Vid. 16 1

6/11/99 1 Little Susitna Vid. 72 1

6/12/99 1 Chickaloon Vid. 1 4

6/12/99 2 Chickaloon Vid. 16 6

6/12/99 4 Beluga R. Vid. 7 1

6/12/99 5 Big Susitna Vid. 31 4

6/12/99 6 Little Susitna Vid. 69 1

6/12/99 7 Knik Arm Vid. 19 3

6/13/99 1 Big Susitna Vid. 16 2

6/13/99 2 Little Susitna Vid. 165 4

6/13/99 4 Knik Arm Vid. 15 5

6/7/00 1 Knik Vid. 18 3

6/7/00 2 Knik Vid. 9 5

6/8/00 1 Little Susitna Vid. 109 8

6/8/00 2 Little Susitna Vid. 22 3

6/8/00 3 Chickaloon Vid. 4 2

6/11/00 1 Little Susitna Vid. 86 9

6/11/00 2 Beluga River Vid. 1 5

6/12/00 1 Chickaloon Vid. 11 8

6/12/00 2 Chickaloon Vid. 4 5

6/12/00 3 Little Susitna Vid. 131 8

6/12/00 4 Little Susitna Vid. 7 1

6/12/00 5 Knik Obs. 5 2

6/12/00 6 Knik Vid. 1 1

6/12/00 7 Knik Vid. 20 5

6/13/00 1 Chickaloon Obs. 4 2

6/13/00 2 Big Susitna Vid. 51 6

6/13/00 3 Knik Obs. 7 2

6/13/00 4 Knik Obs. 3 1

6/13/00 5 Knik Vid. 8.6 5

Correction

Average elapsed for

time during subsurface

count whales

Date Group Location (sec) (A)

6/1/94 1 Big Susitna 17.98 (P) 1.22

6/1/94 2 Big Susitna 14.00 (P) 1.46

6/2/94 1 W of Big Susitna na

6/2/94 2 Turnagain Arm na

6/2/94 3 Chickaloon Bay na

6/3/94 1 Pt Possession 38.00 (O)

6/3/94 2 Kachemak Bay 21.00 (O)

6/3/94 3 Kachemak Bay 25.00 (O)

6/4/94 1 Iniskin Bay 25.67 (O)

6/4/94 2 W of Big Susitna 20.50 (O)

6/4/94 3 Big Susitna 14.97 (P) 1.38

6/4/94 4 W of Little Susitna 11.50/4.60 (P/S) 3.39

6/5/94 1 Pt Possession/E 11.00 (O)

Foreland

6/5/94 2 Beluga R 17.60/8.40 (P/S) 2.21

6/5/94 3 W of Big Susitna 19.80 (P) 1.08

6/5/94 4 W of Big Susitna 17.00 (O)

6/5/94 5 W of Big Susitna 31.47/12.70 (P/S) 1.59

6/5/94 6 Little Susitna 38.50 (O)

6/5/94 7 Chickaloon 65.93/13.38 (P/S) 1.52

7/18/95 1 Chickaloon 15.97 (P) 1.36

7/18/95 2 McArthur R 25.60 (O)

7/18/95 3 Big Susitna 97.71 (O)

7/19/95 1 Chickaloon 17.27 (S) 1.42

7/19/95 2 McArthur R 20.00 (O)

7/19/95 3 Shirleyville na

7/19/95 4 Big Susitna 14.88 (S) 1.72

7/20/95 1 Chickaloon 8.95 (S) 2.11

7/20/95 2 Big Susitna 17.00 (P) 1

7/20/95 3 Big Susitna 56.40 (O)

7/21/95 1 Big Susitna (E) 13.00 (S) 1.55

7/21/95 2 Big Susitna (W) 109.71 (O)

7/21/95 3 Knik Arm na

7/21/95 4 Chickaloon 22.75 (O)

7/22/95 1 Big R 12.29 (S) 1.75

7/24/95 1 Drift R 18.50 (O)

7/24/95 2 McArthur R 11.83 (O)

7/24/95 3 Big Susitna (W) 32.50/7.00 (P/S) 2.54

7/24/95 4 Big Susitna (E) 30.00 (O)

6/11/96 1 S of Beluga R na

6/11/96 2 Lewis R 18.00 (O)

6/11/96 3 Ivan R na

6/11/96 4 Theodore R 20.67 (O)

6/11/96 5 Lewis R 115.33 (O)

6/12/96 1 Knik Arm 17.33 (O)

6/12/96 2 Knik Arm na

6/12/96 3 Big Susitna (after 19.50/8.53 (P/S) 1.64

stranding)

6/12/96 4 Pt Possession na

6/12/96 5 Lewis R 83.14 (O)

6/12/96 6 Theodore R 28.20 (O)

6/12/96 7 Lewis R 25.03 (P) 0.88

6/12/96 8 Big Susitna R 52.50 (O)

6/12/96 9 Big Susitna R 40.75 (O)

6/13/96 1 Knik Arm 25.50 (O)

6/13/96 2 Knik Arm 24.50 (O)

6/13/96 3 Pt Possession 9.58 (S) 2.13

6/13/96 4 Ivan R 36.25 (O)

6/13/96 5 Big Susitna R 7.39 (S) 2.6

6/14/96 1 Pt MacKenzie 24.00 (O)

6/16/96 1 Knik Arm 17.80 (O)

6/16/96 2 Knik Arm 83.60 (O)

6/16/96 3 Pt Possession 6.77 (P/S) 2.32

6/16/96 4 Lewis/Ivan R 5.36 (S) 3.08

6/16/96 5 Big Susitna 11.24 (P/S) 1.75

6/16/96 6 Big/Little Susitna 32.50 (O)

6/16/96 7 Little Susitna 21.74 (P) 1.02

6/17/96 1 Ivan/Big Susitna 5.07 (S) 3.25

6/17/96 3 Little Susitna 18.28 (P) 1.19

6/17/96 4 Ivan/Big Susitna 15.50/5.38 (P/S) 3.06

6/8/97 1 Knik Arm 13.27/4.70 (P/S) 2.14

6/8/97 2 Knik Arm 17.50/12.64 (P/S) 1.41

6/8/97 3 Knik Arm 26.50/9.42 (P/S) 1.8

6/8/97 4 Knik Arm na

6/8/97 5 Knik Arm 8.90 (S) 2.18

6/8/97 7 Knik Arm na

6/8/97 8 Chickaloon 16.20/8.28 (P/S) 2.02

6/8/97 9 Chickaloon 17.12/9.47 (P/S) 1.73

6/8/97 10 Big Susitna 9.30 (S) 2.1

6/9/97 1 Tuxedni Bay na

6/9/97 2 Big Susitna 37.67 (O)

6/10/97 1 Chickaloon 4.87 (S) 3.3

6/10/97 2 Big Susitna 57.75 (O)

6/10/97 3 Knik Arm 9.37 (S) 2.15

6/10/97 4 Knik Arm 26.50/8.06 (P/S) 2.03

6/10/97 5 Knik Arm na

6/10/97 6 Knik Arm na

6/9/98 1 Little Susitna 18.00 (S) 1.18

6/10/98 1 Fire I 13.25 (O)

6/10/98 2 Chickaloon 12.38 (S) 1.62

6/10/98 3 Susitna 16.36 (S) 1.45

6/10/98 4 Knik na

6/10/98 5 Knik 18.00 (O)

6/10/98 6 Knik 15.00 (O)

6/10/98 7 Knik 26.67 (O)

6/10/98 8 Knik 29.00 (O)

6/10/98 9 Knik 31.00 (O)

6/10/98 10 Knik 24.67 (O)

6/12/98 1 Little Susitna 11.74 (S) 1.8

6/12/98 2 Knik 16.75 (O)

6/12/98 3 Knik 12.54 (S) 1.89

6/12/98 4 Knik 11.20 (S) 1.93

6/12/98 5 Knik 7.49 (S) 2.54

6/12/98 6 Knik 64.25 (O)

6/12/98 7 Knik 9.33 (S) 2.04

6/12/98 8 Chickaloon 12.19 (S) 1.74

6/12/98 9 Chickaloon 18.33 (S) 1.16

6/15/98 1 Chickaloon 8.35 (S) 2.34

6/15/98 2 Little Susitna na

6/15/98 3 Little Susitna 11.24 (S) 1.79

6/15/98 4 Knik na

6/15/98 5 Knik 9.69 (S) 2.13

6/15/98 6 Knik 8.20 (S) 2.29

6/15/98 7 Knik 9.51 (S) 2.05

6/9/99 1 Little Susitna 8.05(S) 2.62

6/9/99 2 Knik Arm na

6/9/99 3 Chickaloon 11.60(S) 1.71

6/11/99 1 Little Susitna 5.00(S) 3.21

6/12/99 1 Chickaloon 7.25(S) 2.62

6/12/99 2 Chickaloon 16.76(S) 1.63

6/12/99 4 Beluga R. 14.00(S) 1.46

6/12/99 5 Big Susitna 6.94(S) 2.73

6/12/99 6 Little Susitna 7.33(S) 2.45

6/12/99 7 Knik Arm 13.33(S) 1.69

6/13/99 1 Big Susitna 14.53(S) 1.42

6/13/99 2 Little Susitna 15.41(P) 1.46

6/13/99 4 Knik Arm 22.16(P) 1.35

6/7/00 1 Knik 10.00 (P/S) 1.87

6/7/00 2 Knik 8.40 (S) 2.28

6/8/00 1 Little Susitna 09.12 (P/S) 2.37

6/8/00 2 Little Susitna 18.67 (P/S) 1.17

6/8/00 3 Chickaloon 5.00 (S) 3.32

6/11/00 1 Little Susitna 8.00 (S) 2.37

6/11/00 2 Beluga River 12.40 (S) 1.77

6/12/00 1 Chickaloon 9.25 (S) 2.28

6/12/00 2 Chickaloon 6.60 (S) 2.86

6/12/00 3 Little Susitna 7.50 (S) 2.52

6/12/00 4 Little Susitna 4.00 (S) 3.61

6/12/00 5 Knik na

6/12/00 6 Knik 6.00 (S) 3.00

6/12/00 7 Knik 9.80 (S) 2.00

6/13/00 1 Chickaloon 52.00 (O)

6/13/00 2 Big Susitna 7.67 (S) 2.40

6/13/00 3 Knik 53.50 (O)

6/13/00 4 Knik na

6/13/00 5 Knik 6.00 (S) 2.88

Correction

for whales

missed at

the surface

Date Group Location CV(A) (D) CV(D)

6/1/94 1 Big Susitna 0.13 1.12 0.01

6/1/94 2 Big Susitna 0.22 1.24 0.04

6/2/94 1 W of Big Susitna

6/2/94 2 Turnagain Arm

6/2/94 3 Chickaloon Bay

6/3/94 1 Pt Possession

6/3/94 2 Kachemak Bay

6/3/94 3 Kachemak Bay

6/4/94 1 Iniskin Bay

6/4/94 2 W of Big Susitna

6/4/94 3 Big Susitna 0.17 1.17 0.02

6/4/94 4 W of Little Susitna 0.11 1.14 0.02

6/5/94 1 Pt Possession/E

Foreland

6/5/94 2 Beluga R 0.45 1.16 0.09

6/5/94 3 W of Big Susitna 0.94 1.16 0.09

6/5/94 4 W of Big Susitna

6/5/94 5 W of Big Susitna 0.46 1.19 0.02

6/5/94 6 Little Susitna

6/5/94 7 Chickaloon 1.08 1.16 0.09

7/18/95 1 Chickaloon 0.34 1.26 0.03

7/18/95 2 McArthur R

7/18/95 3 Big Susitna

7/19/95 1 Chickaloon 0.39 1.16 0.04

7/19/95 2 McArthur R

7/19/95 3 Shirleyville

7/19/95 4 Big Susitna 0.12 1.35 0.1

7/20/95 1 Chickaloon 0.38 1.16 0.05

7/20/95 2 Big Susitna 0.45 1.23 0.03

7/20/95 3 Big Susitna

7/21/95 1 Big Susitna (E) 0.61 1.16 0.09

7/21/95 2 Big Susitna (W)

7/21/95 3 Knik Arm

7/21/95 4 Chickaloon

7/22/95 1 Big R 0.41 1.16 0.05

7/24/95 1 Drift R

7/24/95 2 McArthur R

7/24/95 3 Big Susitna (W) 0.16 1.25 0.03

7/24/95 4 Big Susitna (E)

6/11/96 1 S of Beluga R

6/11/96 2 Lewis R

6/11/96 3 Ivan R

6/11/96 4 Theodore R

6/11/96 5 Lewis R

6/12/96 1 Knik Arm

6/12/96 2 Knik Arm

6/12/96 3 Big Susitna (after 0.3 1.16 0.07

stranding)

6/12/96 4 Pt Possession

6/12/96 5 Lewis R

6/12/96 6 Theodore R

6/12/96 7 Lewis R 0.58 1.16 0.07

6/12/96 8 Big Susitna R

6/12/96 9 Big Susitna R

6/13/96 1 Knik Arm

6/13/96 2 Knik Arm

6/13/96 3 Pt Possession 0.22 1.16 0.05

6/13/96 4 Ivan R

6/13/96 5 Big Susitna R 0.1 1.16 0.05

6/14/96 1 Pt MacKenzie

6/16/96 1 Knik Arm

6/16/96 2 Knik Arm

6/16/96 3 Pt Possession 0.32 1.16 0.07

6/16/96 4 Lewis/Ivan R 0.05 1.12 0.01

6/16/96 5 Big Susitna 0.2 1.16 0.05

6/16/96 6 Big/Little Susitna

6/16/96 7 Little Susitna 0.73 1.16 0.07

6/17/96 1 Ivan/Big Susitna 0.08 1.11 0.01

6/17/96 3 Little Susitna 0.42 1.15 0.02

6/17/96 4 Ivan/Big Susitna 0.16 1.16 0.09

6/8/97 1 Knik Arm 0.31 1.1 0.01

6/8/97 2 Knik Arm 0.21 1.29 0.02

6/8/97 3 Knik Arm 0.18 1.17 0.01

6/8/97 4 Knik Arm

6/8/97 5 Knik Arm 0.18 1.18 0.01

6/8/97 7 Knik Arm

6/8/97 8 Chickaloon 0.31 1.19 0.02

6/8/97 9 Chickaloon 0.42 1.19 0.01

6/8/97 10 Big Susitna 0.17 1.21 0.03

6/9/97 1 Tuxedni Bay

6/9/97 2 Big Susitna

6/10/97 1 Chickaloon 0.12 1.14 0.01

6/10/97 2 Big Susitna

6/10/97 3 Knik Arm 0.09 1.14 0.01

6/10/97 4 Knik Arm 0.19 1.18 0.02

6/10/97 5 Knik Arm

6/10/97 6 Knik Arm

6/9/98 1 Little Susitna 0.45 1.13 0.02

6/10/98 1 Fire I

6/10/98 2 Chickaloon 0.31 1.16 0.04

6/10/98 3 Susitna 0.19 1.23 0.02

6/10/98 4 Knik

6/10/98 5 Knik

6/10/98 6 Knik

6/10/98 7 Knik

6/10/98 8 Knik

6/10/98 9 Knik

6/10/98 10 Knik

6/12/98 1 Little Susitna 0.26 1.21 0.02

6/12/98 2 Knik

6/12/98 3 Knik 0.43 1.16 0.07

6/12/98 4 Knik 0.51 1.16 0.05

6/12/98 5 Knik 0.34 1.16 0.05

6/12/98 6 Knik

6/12/98 7 Knik 0.45 1.16 0.09

6/12/98 8 Chickaloon 0.38 1.16 0.05

6/12/98 9 Chickaloon 0.66 1.15 0.02

6/15/98 1 Chickaloon 0.18 1.18 0.02

6/15/98 2 Little Susitna

6/15/98 3 Little Susitna 0.1 1.21 0.01

6/15/98 4 Knik

6/15/98 5 Knik 0.2 1.16 0.04

6/15/98 6 Knik 1.07 1.16 0.07

6/15/98 7 Knik 0.65 1.16 0.04

6/9/99 1 Little Susitna 0.03 1.08 0.00

6/9/99 2 Knik Arm

6/9/99 3 Chickaloon 0.21 1.06 0.00

6/11/99 1 Little Susitna 0.07 1.06 0.00

6/12/99 1 Chickaloon 0.36 1.19 0.14

6/12/99 2 Chickaloon 0.09 1.14 0.02

6/12/99 4 Beluga R. 0.32 1.24 0.00

6/12/99 5 Big Susitna 0.06 1.06 0.00

6/12/99 6 Little Susitna 0.08 1.06 0.00

6/12/99 7 Knik Arm 0.11 1.20 0.00

6/13/99 1 Big Susitna 0.16 1.15 0.00

6/13/99 2 Little Susitna 0.04 1.13 0.00

6/13/99 4 Knik Arm 0.14 1.15 0.00

6/7/00 1 Knik 0.10 1.34 0.15

6/7/00 2 Knik 0.10 1.18 0.05

6/8/00 1 Little Susitna 0.02 1.25 0.07

6/8/00 2 Little Susitna 0.12 1.24 0.05

6/8/00 3 Chickaloon 0.24 1.11 0.03

6/11/00 1 Little Susitna 0.03 1.15 0.04

6/11/00 2 Beluga River 0.35 1.27 0.09

6/12/00 1 Chickaloon 0.08 1.26 0.11

6/12/00 2 Chickaloon 0.16 1.13 0.04

6/12/00 3 Little Susitna 0.02 1.11 0.02

6/12/00 4 Little Susitna 0.22 1.08 0.02

6/12/00 5 Knik

6/12/00 6 Knik 0.61 1.16 0.09

6/12/00 7 Knik 0.07 1.26 0.07

6/13/00 1 Chickaloon

6/13/00 2 Big Susitna 0.04 1.31 0.06

6/13/00 3 Knik

6/13/00 4 Knik

6/13/00 5 Knik 0.10 1.14 0.08

Estimated

CV group

(video size CV([N.

Date Group Location count) ([N.sub.g]) sub.g])

6/1/94 1 Big Susitna 0.11 209 0.17

6/1/94 2 Big Susitna 0.21 263 0.31

6/2/94 1 W of Big Susitna 394 0.6

6/2/94 2 Turnagain Arm 18 0.47

6/2/94 3 Chickaloon Bay 47 0.43

6/3/94 1 Pt Possession 27 0.39

6/3/94 2 Kachemak Bay 8 0.31

6/3/94 3 Kachemak Bay 13 0.29

6/4/94 1 Iniskin Bay 4 0.3

6/4/94 2 W of Big Susitna 144 0.31

6/4/94 3 Big Susitna 0.15 252 0.22

6/4/94 4 W of Little Susitna 0.21 475 0.24

6/5/94 1 Pt Possession/E 2 0.93

Foreland

6/5/94 2 Beluga R 0.21 41 0.51

6/5/94 3 W of Big Susitna 0.21 20 0.96

6/5/94 4 W of Big Susitna 21 0.34

6/5/94 5 W of Big Susitna 0.21 55 0.51

6/5/94 6 Little Susitna 337 0.19

6/5/94 7 Chickaloon 0.21 11 1.1

7/18/95 1 Chickaloon 0.09 29 0.36

7/18/95 2 McArthur R 1 1.15

7/18/95 3 Big Susitna 731 0.36

7/19/95 1 Chickaloon 0.09 20 0.41

7/19/95 2 McArthur R 8 0.58

7/19/95 3 Shirleyville 4 0.42

7/19/95 4 Big Susitna 0.15 348 0.22

7/20/95 1 Chickaloon 0.11 16 0.4

7/20/95 2 Big Susitna 0.21 73 0.5

7/20/95 3 Big Susitna 309 0.41

7/21/95 1 Big Susitna (E) 0.21 32 0.65

7/21/95 2 Big Susitna (W) 278 0.77

7/21/95 3 Knik Arm 2 0.42

7/21/95 4 Chickaloon 43 0.26

7/22/95 1 Big R 0.11 17 0.42

7/24/95 1 Drift R 5 1.05

7/24/95 2 McArthur R 4 0.68

7/24/95 3 Big Susitna (W) 0.21 272 0.27

7/24/95 4 Big Susitna (E) 94 0.45

6/11/96 1 S of Beluga R 2 0.42

6/11/96 2 Lewis R 13 0.69

6/11/96 3 Ivan R 2 0.42

6/11/96 4 Theodore R 9 0.49

6/11/96 5 Lewis R 256 0.25

6/12/96 1 Knik Arm 12 0.39

6/12/96 2 Knik Arm 4 0.42

6/12/96 3 Big Susitna (after 0.15 69 0.34

stranding)

6/12/96 4 Pt Possession 48 0.58

6/12/96 5 Lewis R 304 0.29

6/12/96 6 Theodore R 12 0.39

6/12/96 7 Lewis R 0.15 33 0.6

6/12/96 8 Big Susitna R 199 0.186

6/12/96 9 Big Susitna R 47 0.294

6/13/96 1 Knik Arm 17 0.26

6/13/96 2 Knik Arm 18 0.45

6/13/96 3 Pt Possession 0.12 69 0.26

6/13/96 4 Ivan R 168 0.2

6/13/96 5 Big Susitna R 0.12 229 0.17

6/14/96 1 Pt MacKenzie 39 0.39

6/16/96 1 Knik Arm 37 0.15

6/16/96 2 Knik Arm 28 0.32

6/16/96 3 Pt Possession 0.15 40 0.36

6/16/96 4 Lewis/Ivan R 0.07 365 0.09

6/16/96 5 Big Susitna 0.11 69 0.23

6/16/96 6 Big/Little Susitna 132 0.39

6/16/96 7 Little Susitna 0.15 22 0.74

6/17/96 1 Ivan/Big Susitna 0.15 446 0.17

6/17/96 3 Little Susitna 0.12 30 0.44

6/17/96 4 Ivan/Big Susitna 0.21 230 0.28

6/8/97 1 Knik Arm 0.12 30 0.34

6/8/97 2 Knik Arm 0.09 63 0.23

6/8/97 3 Knik Arm 0.09 76 0.2

6/8/97 4 Knik Arm 2 0.42

6/8/97 5 Knik Arm 0.31 96 0.4

6/8/97 7 Knik Arm 4 0.42

6/8/97 8 Chickaloon 0.09 20 0.32

6/8/97 9 Chickaloon 0.11 19 0.43

6/8/97 10 Big Susitna 0.12 127 0.21

6/9/97 1 Tuxedni Bay 2 0.42

6/9/97 2 Big Susitna 103 0.36

6/10/97 1 Chickaloon 0.11 113 0.16

6/10/97 2 Big Susitna 140 0.35

6/10/97 3 Knik Arm 0.08 153 0.12

6/10/97 4 Knik Arm 0.09 60 0.21

6/10/97 5 Knik Arm 2 0.42

6/10/97 6 Knik Arm 9 0.42

6/9/98 1 Little Susitna 0.21 78 0.5

6/10/98 1 Fire I 21 0.52

6/10/98 2 Chickaloon 0.09 27 0.33

6/10/98 3 Susitna 0.12 139 0.23

6/10/98 4 Knik 4 0.42

6/10/98 5 Knik 21 0.39

6/10/98 6 Knik 7 0.48

6/10/98 7 Knik 8 0.41

6/10/98 8 Knik 64 0.42

6/10/98 9 Knik 9 0.5

6/10/98 10 Knik 49 0.33

6/12/98 1 Little Susitna 0.11 53 0.28

6/12/98 2 Knik 9 0.89

6/12/98 3 Knik 0.15 26 0.46

6/12/98 4 Knik 0.12 18 0.53

6/12/98 5 Knik 0.12 21 0.37

6/12/98 6 Knik 19 0.66

6/12/98 7 Knik 0.21 45 0.51

6/12/98 8 Chickaloon 0.12 31 0.4

6/12/98 9 Chickaloon 0.15 19 0.68

6/15/98 1 Chickaloon 0.12 89 0.22

6/15/98 2 Little Susitna 2 0.42

6/15/98 3 Little Susitna 0.11 285 0.14

6/15/98 4 Knik 21 0.42

6/15/98 5 Knik 0.08 40 0.21

6/15/98 6 Knik 0.15 4 1.08

6/15/98 7 Knik 0.09 11 0.66

6/9/99 1 Little Susitna 0.17 314 0.17

6/9/99 2 Knik Arm 55 0.11

6/9/99 3 Chickaloon 0.40 29 0.46

6/11/99 1 Little Susitna 0.40 245 0.41

6/12/99 1 Chickaloon 0.20 3 0.44

6/12/99 2 Chickaloon 0.17 30 0.19

6/12/99 4 Beluga R. 0.40 13 0.52

6/12/99 5 Big Susitna 0.20 92 0.21

6/12/99 6 Little Susitna 0.40 179 0.41

6/12/99 7 Knik Arm 0.23 39 0.26

6/13/99 1 Big Susitna 0.29 25 0.33

6/13/99 2 Little Susitna 0.20 258 0.21

6/13/99 4 Knik Arm 0.18 18 0.23

6/7/00 1 Knik 0.24 44 0.30

6/7/00 2 Knik 0.19 25 0.22

6/8/00 1 Little Susitna 0.15 317 0.16

6/8/00 2 Little Susitna 0.24 33 0.27

6/8/00 3 Chickaloon 0.30 11 0.38

6/11/00 1 Little Susitna 0.14 231 0.15

6/11/00 2 Beluga River 0.19 2 0.41

6/12/00 1 Chickaloon 0.15 31 0.20

6/12/00 2 Chickaloon 0.19 11 0.25

6/12/00 3 Little Susitna 0.15 357 0.15

6/12/00 4 Little Susitna 0.42 27 0.47

6/12/00 5 Knik 13 0.79

6/12/00 6 Knik 0.42 3 0.74

6/12/00 7 Knik 0.19 49 0.21

6/13/00 1 Chickaloon 9 0.51

6/13/00 2 Big Susitna 0.17 156 0.18

6/13/00 3 Knik 18 0.32

6/13/00 4 Knik 9 0.68

6/13/00 5 Knik 0.19 28 0.22

Table 2.–Paremeters used to estimate beluga whale group sizes based on

the observers’ counts and the time spent counting each group. When

there was no record of time spent counting, the respective aerial count

was multiplied by the simple correction. [b.sub.1,0] is the parameter

used to estimate each observer’s counting performance. [b.sub.2,0] is

the correction of counts as a function of group size. In 2000, none of

the [b.sub.2,0] parameters were significant, so only the simple

corrections were used.

Ob- Simple

ser- correc- [b.sub. [b.sub.

Years ver tion SE 1,0] SE 2,0] SE

1994-98 1 2.01 (0.10) 1.80 (0.13) 0.07 (0.03)

2 2.43 (0.28) 2.88 (1.61) -0.60 (2.11)

3 3.77 (0.38) 2.60 (0.65) 0.17 (0.08)

4 2.84 (0.16) 2.60 (0.39) 0.12 (0.18)

5 1.58 (0.07) 2.25 (0.28) -0.18 (0.07)

6 2.82 (1.64) 2.04 (0.58) 0.41 (0.29)

7 1.43 (0.22) 1.45 (0.70) 0.00 (0.19)

8 2.82 (0.29) 1.59 (0.79) 0.60 (0.36)

1999 1 2.68 (0.16) 2.07 (0.52) 0.24 (0.20)

4 2.19 (0.11) 2.24 (0.33) -0.02 (0.10)

5 2.41 (0.11) 1.97 (0.23) 0.14 (0.07)

8 2.80 (0.17) 1.56 (0.54) 0.56 (0.23)

2000 1 2.45 (0.10)

4 2.90 (0.09)

5 2.87 (0.16)

8 2.83 (0.14)

Acknowledgments

We thank Robyn Angliss, Douglas DeMaster, Barbara Mahoney, Kim Shelden, and Laura Litzky for operating the video camera during aerial surveys. Lisa Baraff, Robin Bush, Suzanne Harkness, Laura Litzky, Marcia Muto, Stephanie Norman, and Kim Shelden counted beluga whales from videotape. Suzanne Harkness helped develop the zoomed video analysis methods and organized and trained video analysts for the 1998 data analysis. Funding for this project was provided by the NMFS Marine Mammal Assessment Program. This research was conducted under Permit No. 791 and 782-1438 issued by the National Marine Fisheries Service. Reviews of this manuscript were provided by Robyn Angliss, Jeff Breiwick, Jeff Laake, Sally Mizroch, Sue Moore, Kim Shelden, and two anonymous reviewers.

(1) Shelden, K. E. W., and R. P. Angliss. 1995. Characterization of beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas) habitat through oceanographic sampling of the Susitna River delta in Cook Inlet, Alaska, 11-18 June 1994. In D. P. DeMaster, H. W. Braham, and P. S. Hill (Editors), Marine Mammal Assessment Program status of stocks and impacts of incidental take, 1994, p. 77-90. Annu. Rep. submitted to Off. Protect. Resour., Natl. Mar. Fish. Serv., NOAA, 1335 East-West Highway, Silver Spring, MD 20910.

(2) Mention of trade names or commercial firms does not imply endorsement by the National Marine Fisheries Service, NOAA.

Literature Cited

Barlow, J., C. W. Oliver, T. D. Jackson, and B. L. Taylor. 1988. Harbor porpoise, Phocoena phocoena, abundance estimation for California, Oregon, and Washington: II. Aerial surveys. Fish. Bull. 86:433-444.

Brodie, P. F. 1971. A reconsideration of aspects of growth, reproduction, and behavior of the white whale (Delphinapterus leucas), with reference to the Cumberland Sound, Baffin Island, population. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 28:1309-1318.

Fraker, M. A. 1980. Status and harvest of the Mackenzie stock of white whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Rep. Int. Whal. Comm. 30:451-458.

Frost, K. J., L. F. Lowry, and R. R. Nelson. 1985. Radiotagging studies of belukha whales (Delphinapterus leucas) in Bristol Bay, Alaska. Mar. Mammal. Sci. 1(3):191-202.

Hobbs, R. C., D. J. Rugh, and D. P. DeMaster. 2000. Abundance of belugas, Delphinapterus leucas, in Cook Inlet, Alaska, 1994-2000. Mar. Fish. Rev. 62(3):37-45.

Kingsley, M. C. S. 1998. Population index estimates for the St. Lawrence belugas, 1973-1995. Mar. Mammal Sci. 14(3):508-530.

Laake, J. L., J. Calambokidis, S. D. Osmek, and G. W. Garner. 1997. Probability of detecting harbor porpoise from aerial surveys: Estimating g(0). J. Wildl. Manage. 61:63-75.

Laidre, K. L., K. E. W. Shelden, D. J. Rugh, and B. A. Mahoney. 2000. Beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, distribution and survey effort in the Gulf of Alaska. Mar. Fish. Rev. 62(3): 27-36.

Lerczak, J. A., K. E. W. Shelden, and R. C. Hobbs. 2000. Application of suction-cup-attached VHF transmitters to the study of beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, surfacing behavior in Cook Inlet, Alaska. Mar. Fish. Rev. 62(3):99-111.

Mahoney, B. A., and K. E. W. Shelden. 2000. Harvest history of belugas, Delphinapterus leucas, in Cook Inlet, Alaska. Mar. Fish. Rev. 62(3): 124-133.

Marsh, H., and D. F. Sinclair. 1989. Correcting for visibility bias in strip transect aerial surveys of aquatic fauna. J. Wildl. Manage. 53(4): 1017-1024.

McLaren, I. A. 1961. Methods of determining the numbers and availability of ringed seals in the eastern Canadian Arctic. Arctic 14(3):162-175.

Moore, S. E., K. E. W. Shelden, L. K. Litzky, B. A. Mahoney, and D. J. Rugh. 2000. Beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, habitat associations in Cook Inlet, Alaska. Mar. Fish. Rev. 62(3):60-80.

Rugh, D. J., K. E. W. Shelden, and B. A. Mahoney. 2000. Distribution of belugas, Delphinapterus leucas, in Cook Inlet, Alaska, during June/July 1993-2000. Mar. Fish. Rev. 62(3): 6-21.

Sergeant, D. E. 1973. Biology of white whales (Delphinapterus leucas) in western Hudson Bay. J. Fish. Res. Board Can. 30:1065-1090.

The authors are with the National Marine Mammal Laboratory, Alaska Fisheries Science Center, National Marine Fisheries Service, NOAA, 7600 Sand Point Way N.E., Seattle, WA 98115-0070 (e-mail address: rod.hobbs@noaa.gov).

COPYRIGHT 2000 U.S. Department of Commerce

COPYRIGHT 2004 Gale Group

You May Also Like

A Decline in Starfish, Asterias forbesi, Abundance and a Concurrent Increase in Northern Quahog, Mercenaria mercenaria, Abundance and Landings in the Northeastern United States

A Decline in Starfish, Asterias forbesi, Abundance and a Concurrent Increase in Northern Quahog, Mercenaria mercenaria, Abundance and Landings i…

Abundance of belugas, Delphinapterus leucas, in Cook Inlet, Alaska, 1994-2000

Abundance of belugas, Delphinapterus leucas, in Cook Inlet, Alaska, 1994-2000 Roderick C. Hobbs Introduction Beluga, Delphin…

Humpback and fin whaling in the Gulf of Maine from 1800 to 1918

Humpback and fin whaling in the Gulf of Maine from 1800 to 1918 Randall R. Reeves Introduction The Gulf of Maine is an ocean…

William Francis Thompson and the dawn of marine fisheries research in California

William Francis Thompson and the dawn of marine fisheries research in California J. Richard Dunn Introduction As concern inc…